TY - JOUR
T1 - Attributing effects to a cluster-randomized get-out-the-vote Campaign
AU - Hansen, Ben B.
AU - Bowers, Jake
N1 - Funding Information:
Ben Hansen is Assistant Professor, Statistics Department, University of Michigan, Ann Arbor, MI 48109 (E-mail: [email protected]). Jake Bowers is Assistant Professor, Department of Political Science and NCSA, University of Illinois at Urbana–Champaign, Urbana, IL 61801 (E-mail: [email protected]). This work was supported in part by The Robert Wood Johnson Foundation and by National Science Foundation grant DMS-0102056. The authors are grateful for helpful discussions arising from several seminars in which they presented parts of this work. They also thank Michael Elliott, Donald Green, Kosuke Imai, Andrew Gelman, Roderick Little, David Nickerson, Shawn Treier, two anonymous referees, an anonymous associate editor, and the editor for helpful comments and suggestions. Any errors or shortcomings are their own.
PY - 2009
Y1 - 2009
N2 - Early in the twentieth century, Fisher and Neyman demonstrated how to infer effects of agricultural interventions using only the very weakest of assumptions, by randomly varying which plots were to be manipulated. Although the methods permitted uncontrolled variation between experimental units, they required strict control over assignment of interventions; this hindered their application to field studies with human subjects, who ordinarily could not be compelled to comply with experimenters' instructions. In 1996, however, Angrist, Imbens, and Rubin showed that inferences from randomized studies could accommodate noncompliance without significant strengthening of assumptions. Political scientists A. Gerber and D. Green responded quickly, fielding a randomized study of voter turnout campaigns in the November 1998 general election. Noncontacts and refusals were frequent, but Gerber and Green analyzed their data in the style of Angrist et al., avoiding the need to model nonresponse. They did use models for other purposes: to address complexities of the randomization scheme; to permit heterogeneity among voters and campaigners; to account for deviations from experimental protocol; and to take advantage of highly informative covariates. Although the added assumptions seemed straightforward and unassailable, a later analysis by Imai found them to be at odds with Gerber and Green's data. Using a different model, he reaches the very opposite of Gerber and Green's central conclusion about getting out the vote. This article shows that neither of the models are necessary, addressing all of the complications of Gerber and Green's study using methods in the tradition of Fisher and Neyman. To do this, it merges recent developments in randomization-based inference for comparative studies with somewhat older developments in design-based analysis of sample surveys. The method involves regression, but large-sample analysis and simulations demonstrate its lack of dependence on regression assumptions. Its substantive results have consequences both for the design of campaigns to increase voter participation and for theories of political behavior more generally.
AB - Early in the twentieth century, Fisher and Neyman demonstrated how to infer effects of agricultural interventions using only the very weakest of assumptions, by randomly varying which plots were to be manipulated. Although the methods permitted uncontrolled variation between experimental units, they required strict control over assignment of interventions; this hindered their application to field studies with human subjects, who ordinarily could not be compelled to comply with experimenters' instructions. In 1996, however, Angrist, Imbens, and Rubin showed that inferences from randomized studies could accommodate noncompliance without significant strengthening of assumptions. Political scientists A. Gerber and D. Green responded quickly, fielding a randomized study of voter turnout campaigns in the November 1998 general election. Noncontacts and refusals were frequent, but Gerber and Green analyzed their data in the style of Angrist et al., avoiding the need to model nonresponse. They did use models for other purposes: to address complexities of the randomization scheme; to permit heterogeneity among voters and campaigners; to account for deviations from experimental protocol; and to take advantage of highly informative covariates. Although the added assumptions seemed straightforward and unassailable, a later analysis by Imai found them to be at odds with Gerber and Green's data. Using a different model, he reaches the very opposite of Gerber and Green's central conclusion about getting out the vote. This article shows that neither of the models are necessary, addressing all of the complications of Gerber and Green's study using methods in the tradition of Fisher and Neyman. To do this, it merges recent developments in randomization-based inference for comparative studies with somewhat older developments in design-based analysis of sample surveys. The method involves regression, but large-sample analysis and simulations demonstrate its lack of dependence on regression assumptions. Its substantive results have consequences both for the design of campaigns to increase voter participation and for theories of political behavior more generally.
KW - Cluster randomization
KW - Group randomized trial
KW - Instrumental variable
KW - Model-assisted
KW - Randomization inference
KW - Voter turnout
UR - http://www.scopus.com/inward/record.url?scp=69149096087&partnerID=8YFLogxK
UR - http://www.scopus.com/inward/citedby.url?scp=69149096087&partnerID=8YFLogxK
U2 - 10.1198/jasa.2009.ap06589
DO - 10.1198/jasa.2009.ap06589
M3 - Article
AN - SCOPUS:69149096087
SN - 0162-1459
VL - 104
SP - 873
EP - 885
JO - Journal of the American Statistical Association
JF - Journal of the American Statistical Association
IS - 487
ER -